Disclaimer: This literature review has been written by a student and is not an example of our professional work, which you can see examples of here.

Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this literature review are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of UKDiss.com.

Preferences for Female Political Leaders in Times of Threat

Info: 9699 words (39 pages) Example Literature Review
Published: 17th Jan 2022

Reference this

Tagged: PoliticsLeadership

Abstract

Women are underrepresented in the United States government despite accounting for approximately half of the U.S. electorate. Therefore, investigation into the types of contexts in which female candidates may emerge may improve gender parity. This study seeks to investigate preferences for female candidates in times of threat, as evidence from the glass cliff literature indicates that female leaders may be preferred to male leaders in times of crisis, that this preference may be driven, in part, by system justifying ideologies (Brown, Diekman, & Schneider, 2011), and that the type of threat may influence preferences for female leaders.

Therefore, I propose to investigate preferences for female political leaders as a function of the membership of the group posing the threat and whether the leader is described as upholding cultural traditions. University students will read vignettes about a terrorist attack perpetrated by ingroup member (i.e., White supremacists) or outgruop members (ISIS) and then view responses from male and female presidential primary contenders who either voice support for cultural traditions or do not mention them. Participants then rate their perceptions of the candidates’ warmth and competence and indicate how likely they would be to vote for the candidate in the primary and how electable they believe the candidate to be in a general election.

In the 2016 U.S. presidential election, Donald Trump beat presumed frontrunner, Hillary Clinton, after a bruising political campaign. In her concession speech, Clinton directly addressed those who had hoped to see a female president in their lifetime, stating, “I know we have still not shattered that highest and hardest glass ceiling, but some day someone will and hopefully sooner than we might think right now” (Clinton, 2016). Certainly, there are many potential reasons why Clinton’s campaign was unsuccessful. Some have blamed an overemphasis on identity politics (Lilla, 2016), whereas others see Trump’s win as a repudiation of the policies of the Bush and Obama administrations (Guo, 2016). However, some, including Clinton herself (e.g., Suliman, 2017) have attributed her loss at least partially to sexism, prompting some to wonder more generally under what conditions a female political leader might be desirable.

The purpose of the proposed research is to investigate conditions under which a female U.S. presidential candidate would be preferred to a male U.S presidential candidate among American college students. One condition in which a female head of state might be desirable is when the nation is under threat, as evidence suggests women are more likely to be nominated to precarious leadership positions when organization is in crisis (Ryan & Haslam, 2005) and that this phenomenon may be motivated by system justifying ideologies (Brown, Diekman, & Schneider, 2011).

It also seems likely that this effect likely also depends on the type of threat, as some studies (e.g., Bruckmüller & Branscombe, 2010) show a diminished preference for a male leader, whereas others (e.g, Brown et al., 2011) show a pronounced preference for a female leader. I therefore investigate preferences for male and female candidates as a function of system justification (i.e., whether the candidate is described  as endorsing cultural tradiitons or not), and the type of threat, that is whether the threat comes from members of the ingroup (i.e., domestic terrorism) or members of the outgroup (i.e., international terrorism).

Gender Roles

Social Role Theory

In order to understand the machanisms that might motivate preferences for female leaders, it is necessary to consider how gender roles and stereotypes affect perceptions of women who work, especially in stereotypically masculine professions. Social role theory (Eagly, Wood, & Diekman, 2000) suggests that women and men are ascribed traits that correspond toon their representation in different occupational roles. To the extent that women are seen in caretaking roles (e.g., occupations such as teachers, nurses, homemakers), they are ascribed communal traits such as being warm, kind, and nurturing. Similarly, to the extent that men are seen in breadwinning roles (e.g., occupations such as business leaders, doctors, lawyers), they are ascribed agentic traits, such as being dynamic, assertive, and aggressive. Thus social role theory encompasses descriptive gender role content, that is, traits that are typical of men and women

Several studies indicate support for this theory, For example, in a series of studies, Diekman and Eagly (2000) demonstrated that perceptions of the traits that men and women have vary as a function of occupational role. For example, women were perceived as having more agentic traits over time, consistent with women’s increasing participation in the labor force since 1950. There is also evidence suggesting that social role theory may apply more generally to perceptions of different groups (e.g., ethnic, racial, and religious groups). Koenig and Eagly (2014) found that perceptions of group traits for different racial, ethnic and religious groups varied as a function of their occupational roles. In other words, regardless of whether the target group is gender or another group (e.g., religious groups), representation in occupational roles appears to impact perceptions of the types of traits that people in that group may typically posess.

Role Congruity Theory

Of course, judgments of group members not only depend on individuals’ perceptions of traits that groups are likely to have (i.e., descriptive stereotypes) but also judgments of the types of traits that group members should have (i.e., prescriptive stereotypes). Therefore, social role theory was augmented by role congruity theory (Eagly & Karau, 2002), which describes the prescriptive content of gender roles. According to role congruity theory, individuals whose actions do not align with their prescribed gender roles are be judged harshly or punished for role incongruity, whereas those who behave in ways that are consistent with gender roles are rewared.

Indeed, a great deal of evidence supports the propositions made in role congruity theory. For example, judgments of male and female job applicants who were described as either self promoting (i.e., displaying agentic behaviors) or self-effacing (i.e., displaying communal behaviors) indicated that self-promoting candidates were generally preferred to those who were self-effacing. However, female candidates who exhibited self-promoting behaviors were judged as having fewer social skills and as being less hireable than their male colleagues (Rudman, 1998), suggesting that women may be punished for role-incongruent behavior, even if that behavior is generally viewed positively.

Evidence also suggests that conformity to gender roles is rewarded. For exampole, evidence suggests that young children are rewarded for conforming to gender roles (Bussey & Bandura, 1999) and that among individuals who placed importance on gender roles, gender role-congruent behavior was associated with greater self-esteem and smaller discrepancies between ought and ideal selves (Wood, Chistensen, Hebl, & Rothgerber, 1997). Therefore, it appears that self-regulating mechanisms and societal mechanisms reward individuals for engaging in role-congruent behaviors.

Gender and Leadership

Female Leadership Disadvantage

Role congruity theory makes two specific predictions about female leaders. In particular, it describes a “double-bind” for female leaders (Eagly & Karau, 2002). The first part of this bind describes how women may be passed over for promotion to leadership positions because descriptive theories of gender roles do not align with perceptions of leaders. That is, individuals may assume that because women are less commonly found in leadership positions, they are less qualified for them than are men. The second part of the bind describes the experiences of women already in leadership positions. As leadership may require displays of agency, women in leadership may be judged harshly for violation of prescriptive gender norms. Therefore female leaders may be regarded as cold, cutthroat or manipulative.

Indeed, research on gender and leadership generally supports the double bind described by role congruity theory. For example, meta-analytic evidence suggests that stereotypes of men align more closely with stereotypes of leaders than do stereotyeps of women across three different paradigms measuring aspects of agency and communion (Koenig, Eagly, Mitchell, & Ristikari, 2011). Similar research on implicit leadership theories (ILTs) indicates that protytypes of successful leaders are closer to prototypes of men than prototypes of women (Hall, Workman, & Marchioro, 1998) and that leadership prototypes are more descriptive of masculine than feminine traits (Schein, 1973).

Furthermore, there is evidence that man and women may be held to different standards in leadership positions. For example, meta-analytic evidence from 61 studies suggests that perceptions of leaders are more positive when the leader is male rather than female, particularly when judged by men (vs. women) and when the leader was described as displaying agentic qualities, such as being directive (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992). There is also evidence suggesting that leader behaviors may be judged differently depending on the gender of the leader. For example, leaders exhibiting both masculine and feminine traits were judged more positively than those exhibiting only masculine or feminine qualities.

However, this was only the case if the leader was described as a man instead of a woman (Wolfram & Gratton, 2013). Similarly, Kark, Waismel-Manor, and Shamir (2012) found that transformational leadership styles were associated with masculine and feminine qualities. However, women were judged more harshly for failing to embody these qualities than were men. Thus, there is evidence that female leaders may be held to a higher standard than are male leaders.

Political Leaders

Research on female politicians, in particular, similarly suggests that women may be disadvantaged in leadership positions. For example, in two studes, one experimental and one using data from incumbant elections from the 2000 and 2004 congressional elections, Bauer (2015) demonstrated that the presensence of feminine stereotypes in political ads was associated with lower ratings of female (vs. male) leaders’ potential effectivenss as a senator and fitness as a presidential candidate. She also found that the presence of feminie stereotypes in ads was associated with a decreased likelihood of voting for a female versus male candidate. Other work similarly suggests that female leaders may be perceived as unqualified for political offices because political offices (e.g., president, senator) may not be associated with characteristics typically ascribed to women (Schneider & Bos, 2014).

Additionally, theer is some evidence that both parts of the double-bind described by role congruity theory may affect perceptions of female political candidates. For example, Gervais and Hillard (2008) found in a study on perceptions of Sarah Palin and Hillary Clinton that Clinton was perceived as less feminine and warm but more competent than was Palin, suggesting that perceptions of both candidates may have, in part, been driven by perception of how closely each candidate’s behavior aligned with stereotypically feminie gender roles. Additionally, Gervais and Hillard found that the probabiliby of voting for Palin over Clinton was much stronger among participants who strongly endorsed gender role traditionalism, suggesting that participants’ voting behavior may, in part, be motivated by whether female candidates’ behaviors align with prescribed gender roles. Therefore, it is possible that female political candidates who display agency may be viewed as competent but unlikeable, whereas those who display communion may be well-liked by risk being viewed as incompetent.

Female Leadership Advantage

However, despite definite disadvantages facing female leaders, there are some instances in which female candidates may be advantaged relative to their male colleagues.  For example, some evidence suggests that because women face more barriers to leadership roles than do men, female leaders may be perceived as more qualified than male leaders in the same position (Foschi, 2000). Indeed, Rosette and Trost (2010) found that participants judged female leaders who displayed agentic behaviors as more effective than male leaders in top management positions. However this effect only emerged when female leaders’ success could be internally attributed.Thus, under some circumstances individuals may view female leaders as being more successful than male leaders.

Other evidence suggests that preferences for female leaders may emerge in leadership positions that complement feminine stereotypes. For example, meta-analytic evidence suggests that female leaders may be preferred in industries such as education and in positions such as middle management, which may require more communication skills than upper-management positions (Paustian-Underdahl, Walker, & Woeher, 2014). Similarly, there is also evidence that a female leadership advantage might exist because women are more likely than men to engage leadership behaviors such as transformational (Vinkenburg, van Engen, Eagly, & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2011) and democratic leadership (Eagly & Carli, 2003). Therefore, it is possible that despite negative perceptions of women in leadership positions, there may be some instances in which women are more likely to excel in leadership positions than are men.

Political Leaders

Research on female political leaders similarly suggests that female politicans may benefit from being women in some specific circumstances. For example, female politicians were preferred when issues such as education and health care were important to participants (Dolan, 2010). Similarly, evidence suggests that women may be preferred at lower levels of government, where issues that are more consistent with feminine steroetypes (e.g., education) are more salient (Kahn, 1994). Other evidence indicates that female leaders may be less suceptible to negative campaign ads than are male candiates (Fridkin, Kenny, & Woodall, 2008),suggesting that despite the fact that political offices are ssociated with agency, there are some situations in which female politicians may be preferred to male politicians.

The Glass Cliff

There is also another context in which women might be preferred to men. Some evidence that there is a selection bias that favors women over men when leadership positions are precarious (e.g., when the organization is in crisis), known as the “glass cliff phenomenon.” Ryan and Haslam (2005) coined the “glass cliff” after demonstrating that historically, women were disproportionately nominated to organizational boards after dips in the organization’s performance and stock prices. Evidence for the glass cliff has been demonstrated in both archival (e.g., Ryan & Haslam) and experimental data (e.g., Brown et al., 2011).  It is important to note that the glass cliff effect is generally considered a phenomenon rather than a theory (Ryan, Haslam, Morgentroth, Rink, Stoker, & Peters, 2016). That is, the class cliff is a phenomenon is an effect that can be observed, which can be explained through other theories such as role congruity theory.

This may be one reason that mixed evidence has been found for its existence across studies. For example, although numerous studies have demonstrated the glass cliff phenomenon (e.g., Ryan & Haslam, 2005; Haslam & Ryan, 2008; Ryan, Haslam, Hersby, & Bongiorno, 2011), others studies have not. For example, in an examination of U.S. Fortune 500 there was not evidence of a gender difference in CEO appointments when examining objective accountancy-based measures of company performance (Adams, Gupta, & Leeth, 2009). Other studies have similarly failed to find evidence of the glass cliff phenomenon among and Canadian samples (Carroll, Hennessey, & MacDonald 2013). One potential explanation for this discrepancy is the way in which performance is measured. For example, accountancy-based measures were unrelated to preference for a male or female leader. However, when performance was conceptualized in terms of stock-based performance, female leaders were preferred to male leader (Haslam, Ryan, Kulich, Trojanowski, & Atkins, 2010). Thus, it is possible that preferences for female leaders when an organization is in crisis may have more to do with perceived performance than actual performance.

The Glass Cliff and Political Leaders

In any case, one area of glass cliff research that has received relatively little attention is the role of the glass cliff effect in the election of female leaders. Using archival data from the 2005 UK general election, Ryan, Haslam, and Kulich (2010) demonstrated that women ran for less winnable seats than did men, and this was particularly pronounced for the minority party (i.e., for Conservative women) than it was for the majority party (i.e., Labour). Thus, there appears to be at least some archival evidence suggesting that female political candidates more often run for precarious seats than do male political candidates. Some evidence of the glass cliff also exists in experimental studies. Ryan et al. (2010) found that in a laboratory study using British political science students, participants showed a preference for a female candidate when a seat was described as difficult to win but a preference for a male candidate when the seat was described as winnable.

One potential criticism of Ryan et al.’s study is that although they demonstrated that there is a preference for female political leaders when the seat is precarious, that does not necessarily mean that there is a preference for female political leaders when the country is in crisis (as might be the case for a precarious leadership position in an organization). Therefore, it is possible that individuals may prefer women for precarious leadership seats, but not when the nation is under threat. Indeed, some evidence suggests that male political candidates are preferred to women on issues of war and national security (e.g., Huddy & Terkildsen, 1993). However, other evidence suggests that there is an increased preference for female political leaders in times of threat. In two separate studies, Brown et al. (2011) demonstrated the glass cliff effect in two studies. In response to being primed about threats in their communities, participants’ preference for a male leader relative to a female leader was eliminated, and in response to a prime about the 9/11 terrorist attacks, participants preferred a female leader relative to a male leader. These studies suggest that not only do individuals prefer female political candidates when the election is precarious but that the preference for a female political leader may occur when the country is in crisis or under threat.

Therefore, I predict the following:

H1: I predict that there will be a preference for a female leader relative to a male leader when participants are primed with a threat.

Causes of the Glass Cliff

Stereotypes

There are a few explanations that might explain the glass cliff phenomenon. The first is that stereotypes of leaders in times of crisis may align more closely with stereotypes of women than stereotypes of men. For example, Ryan, Haslam, and Bongiorno (2011) codified the “think crisis think-female” effect, showing that stereotypes of leaders when a firm was in economic crisis were more consistent with stereotypes of women than stereotypes of men. Other work (e.g., Gartzia, Ryan, Balluerka, & Aritzeta, 2012) similarly suggests that consistent with role congruity theory, individuals may prefer leaders whose traits align with organizational roles, in this case, traits of a leader in a time of crisis. Indeed, this research is consistent with research on ILTs that suggests that, prototypes of typical leaders differ from those of ideal leaders (Junker & van Dick, 2014). It might therefore be possible that ILTs of a leader in charge of an organization in crisis might differ from those in an organization that is thriving.

Selection Bias

There is also some evidence that the glass cliff effect might depend somewhat on participant gender. For example, there is some evidence that when an organization is described as in crisis, women preferred feminine traits to masculine traits more than did men, whereas men did not differentially prefer feminine or masculine traits (Ryan et al., 2011). Other evidence similarly indicates that women may be more partial to female leaders, especially in times of crisis. However, it is also important to note that these gender effects are not consistent across studies, as others (e.g., Haslam & Ryan, 2008; Ryan et al., 2010) did not find that the glass cliff phenomenon depended on participant gender.

Additionally, Brown et al. (2011) did not find that preference for a female versus male leader in times of crisis depended participant gender, although they found that women more strongly associated women with change than did men. Therefore, the effects of participant gender on the glass cliff effect are somewhat unclear. I therefore propose to include participant gender as a factor in my experimental design so as to control for its potential effects. However, I make no formal hypotheses about the role of gender in preference for a female candidate when the nation is under threat.

Glass Cliff and System Justification

Some work on the glass cliff work indicates that women may also be nominated to precarious leadership positions as a means to uphold the status quo (Ryan & Haslam, 2005). Indeed, Ryan et al. (2011) found that one of the reasons that women were nominated to leadership positions is that women are expected to take responsibility for the crisis and therefore may serve as a scapegoat for the failure of the company. The potential for female leaders in precarious positions to fail also reaffirms stereotypes that women cannot be leaders, justifying existing social systems in which men are more likely to be found in high-status roles than are women. Therefore, it is possible that the nomination of a female leader may be a way to maintain rather than upend existing social structures and it is therefore possible that the preference for a female (vs. male) leader in times of crisis might be driven by system-justifying motives.

System justifying ideologies allow individuals to believe that existing political, economic, and social systems are fair and legitimate, regardless of whether individuals benefit from these systems (Jost & Hunyady, 2005). Examples of such ideologies include social dominance orientation (Ho, Sidanius, Pratto, Levin, Kteily, & Sheehy-Skeffinton), the belief that some groups (e.g., Whites, men) are inherently superior to other groups (e.g., Blacks, women). Other examples of system justifying ideologies include political conservatism and right-wing authoritarianism, both of which involve a preference for greater societal traditionalism (e.g., Jost & Thompson, 2000). Among the consequents of such ideologies are an increased support for societal institutions and a decreased support for social change (Jost & Hunyady, 2005). In other words, individuals who strongly system-justifying ideologies have a strong need to maintain the status quo.

Sometimes a change in leadership might be desirable in order to maintain the existing social system. Donald Trump’s election might be an example of such a change. For example, although some argue that Trump voters were motivated by a desire to change policies that favored an increasingly globalized economy (e.g., Tumulty, Rucker, & Gearan, 2016), others argued that Donald Trump’s election and the election of similar populist figures is a backlash against social change (e.g., Inglehart & Norris, 2016). For example, it is possible that some voters considered eight years of a Black president and the possibility of a female president to be too much change to existing social structures in which Whites and men enjoy higher social status than do Black and women, respectively. Therefore Trump’s election potentially signaled a return to the status quo rather than a change away from the existing system.

In a study on the effects of system justification on the outcomes of the 2016 presidential election, Azvedo, Jost, and Rothmnd (2017) found support indicating that Trump’s election may have been motivated by a need to return to the status quo. Although they found that general system justifying ideologies were associated with stronger support for Hillary Clinton (perhaps supporting claims by some that she was the “status quo” candidate), strong support for economic and gender system justification were associated with greater support for Donald Trump. Indeed, despite Trump’s criticism of an increasingly globalized economy, Trump supporters showed an increased preference for the types of social structures that drove those inequalities. Therefore, the desire to “Make American Great Again” may have served to signal a return to the status quo rather than a departure from it.

These results make a case that in some cases, change (e.g., the election of a new political candidate) may serve to uphold rather than change the existing social system. In this sense, it is possible that a female candidate may be viewed as a form of change that may serve to justify existing social systems rather than change them. Indeed, Brown et al. (2011) found support for this hypothesis, demonstrating that the tendency to prefer a female candidate relative to a male candidate in times of crisis was especially pronounced among individuals high in system justifying ideologies and social dominance orientation. Other evidence suggests that the presence of female candidates may cue system-justifying ideologies. For example, in a series of three experiments Brown and Diekman (2013) demonstrated that the presence of female politicians increased system justification. In particular, they showed that the presence of female candidates lead to greater perceptions of fairness, a greater acceptance of gender inequality, and a preference for the constancy rather than change. Therefore, counter-intuitively, although female candidates may represent a change in the sense that historically, U.S. presidents have been male, female candidates in some ways may represent a return to the status quo.

It is likely that given that individuals are generally motivated to perceive the system is fair that candidates who are perceived as upholding traditional American values (consistent system justifying ideologies such as political conservatism) will be more electable and more likeable than candidates who are not perceived as upholding traditions. Furthermore, given the fact that preferences for female candidates may be driven by system justifying ideologies, it is possible that preference for a female candidate may be especially strong when she is perceived as upholding the system (i.e., upholding cultural traditions) than when she is perceived as not upholding cultural traditions. Therefore, I predict the following:

H2: Candidates who endorse cultural traditions will be viewed more positively than candidates who are not viewed as upholding cultural traditions.

H3: I further expect this effect to be qualified by candidate gender such that the preference for a female versus a male candidate in times of threat will be stronger when the candidate is portrayed as upholding cultural traditions versus not mentioning cultural traditions.

Type of Threat

There is also some evidence from the glass cliff literature that the type of threat may affect preference for a female leader. For example, effects in the glass cliff literature are unclear as to whether threats diminish the preference for a male candidate relative to a female candidate or increase the presence for a female candidate relative to a male candidate. For example, a distinct preference for a female leader was found when threat was primed with an unsuccessful company (Haslam & Ryan, 2008) or poor financial performance (Ryan et al., 2011). However, in response to a crisis, Bruckmüller and Branscombe (2010) merely found a decreased preference for male leaders.

Effects across Brown and Diekman’s (2011) experiments were inconsistent as well. For example, when participants were asked to write about threats in their local communities (i.e., economic instability, violent crime, or job cuts), preferences for a male leader decreased in the threat (vs. control) condition. However preference for a female leader remained unchanged. However, in response to threat primed by the 9/11 terrorist attacks, participants preferred a female leader to a male leader. One explanation for these effects is that not just the mere presence of the threat but the type of threat might affect preference for a female leader.

One such type of threat that might affect the preference for a female (vs. male) leader might be whether the threat to the system comes from an ingroup member (e.g., White supremacist group) rather than an outgroup member (e.g., ISIS), as threats from outgroup members may be perceived as more threatening to the status quo than threats posed by an ingroup member. For example, non-Black American participants were quicker to associate a Black face with a gun than a White face and more often mistook a tool for a gun when presented with a Black face rather than a White face (Payne, 2001). This tendency is also consistent evidence in the United States indicating that although Americans are extremely unlikely to die in an attack perpetrated by a foreign-born terrorist (Nowraseth, 2016) and that nearly half of the deaths from terrorism can be attributed to domestic rather than foreign groups (Willis, 2017), the media often portrays foreign terrorism as a much more serious threat than domestic terrorism (Powell, 2011).

It is therefore possible that threats from outgroup members may produce a greater desire for a protection of the existing system than might threats from ingroup members. Therefore, if system justifying mechanisms produce a preference for a female leader in times of crisis, preference for a female leader may increase when a threat is posed by an outgroup member than an ingroup member. Furthermore, this effect might be stronger if the leader is perceived as upholding cultural traditions (i.e., representing a return to the status quo) rather than not mentioning cultural traditions.

I therefore predict the following:

H4: Preference for a female leader relative to a male leader will be stronger when the terrorist attack is described as coming from an outgroup member (i.e., ISIS) than an ingroup member (i.e., a White supremacist group).

H5: There will be a three-way interaction between target gender, mention of traditionalism, and threat type such that preference for a female leader will be stronger when the threat is posed by an ingroup member than an outgroup member, and this effect will be stronger when the candidate endorses traditions (vs. no mention).

Study Overview

The purpose of this research was to investigate the glass cliff phenomenon in a political setting in which preferences political candidates may vary as a function of threat (i.e., posed by ingroup vs. outgroup member), and whether that candidate was described as endorsing traditions (i.e., upholding the status quo) or not. Participants were asked to read an online newspaper article describing a terrorist attack perpetrated by either ISIS (i.e., outgroup member) or a White supremacist group (i.e., ingroup member). Next participants were directed to watch two videos of two ostensive presidential primary candidates (one male and one female) responding to the attacks and whose political affiliation matched that of the participant.

In their response, the candidates either endorsed traditions or did not endorse traditions. Participants then indicated their perceptions of the candidate, including how warmly the felt towards the candidate, how likely they were to vote for the candidate, how fit the candidate was for office, and how much they though the candidate would win in the general election. Finally, although I made no formal predictions involving participant gender, I propose to keep it as a factor in the design, as some studies have found that the glass cliff effect depends on participant gender. Thus, I have proposed a 2 (Target Gender) X 2 (Threat Type: Ingroup vs. Outgroup) X 2 (System Justification: Mention vs. No Mention) X 2 (Participant Gender) design with repeated measures of the first factor.

Overall, I expect that there will be a general preference for a female leader to a male leader, as all participants were primed with threat. I also expect a main effect of traditionalism such that participants will generally prefer a candidate who upholds tradition to one that does not. I further expect that preference for a female leader will be stronger when the threat to the group comes from an outgroup member (i.e., ISIS) rather than an ingroup member (i.e., ISIS), as an ougroup threat constitutes a larger threat to the existing system and may potentially prime a greater desire to return to the status quo.

Finally, I expect a three-way interaction between target gender, threat type, and traditionalism, such that preference for a female leader will be stronger when the threat comes from outside (vs. within) of the group, particularly when the female leader is described as upholding cultural traditions (vs. no mention of traditionalism).

Pilot Study

I propose to pilot the videos of the candidates as well as the candidates’ scripts to ensure that any of the effects of the study are not due to extraneous variables such as candidate attractiveness.

Method

Participants

I propose to use 50 participants from a small mid-Western university.

Materials and Procedure

Four videos will be made of men and women talking for thirty seconds about their favorite television show. Two targets of each gender will be included to ensure that no extraneous variables particular to target might affect results. Participants will be called into the lab for a study ostensibly on first impressions and will be asked to watch videos of one man and one women (order counterbalanced across participants) and rate their perceptions how attractive each speaker is and how likeable each speaker is on a 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much) scale. Thus, the design is a 2 (Target Gender) X 2 (Target A vs. Target B) mixed model design with repeated measures on the first factor.

After judging videos participants will be routed to a presumably unrelated study on political speeches. As participants in the main study will watch two clips of political candidates, slightly different (but equivalent) versions of each response (upholding vs. not holding traditions) will be necessary in order to be believable.

Therefore, I propose to write four responses to a terrorist attack, two where candidates are described as upholding traditions and two in which they are not. The text of one of the responses in which participants were described as upholding traditions is described below.

Our thoughts and prayers go out to the families affected by the recent attacks. I want to promise the American people that if elected, my administration will stop at nothing to make sure perpetrators of such attacks are hunted down and swiftly brought to justice. We stand by those who were hurt and lost loved ones today and will be a source of comfort to them in their darkest hour. Let us remember that terror can never win against the American traditions and values that I pledge to uphold and which make this country so great. Thank you and may God bless America.

In the no mention condition, the candidate will state that terror an never win against the will and spirit of the American people, which are indomitable and which make this country so great.

Participants will read two of the four vignettes: one in which candidates were described as upholding traditions and one in which they are not. Thus, the design is a 2 (Version A vs. Version B) X 2 (Traditionalism: Mention vs. No Mention) design with repeated measures on the last factor. After each vignette, participants will indicate how effective they thought each statement was how caring they believed each statement was, and how presidential they thought each statement was on a 1 (Not at all) to 7 (Very much) likert-type scale.

Proposed Results and Discussion

Participants’ ratings of attractiveness and likeability of videos of candidates bill be submitted to a 2 (Target Gender) X 2 (Version A vs. Version B) mixed model ANOVA with repeated measures on the first factor. In order to move forward, perceptions of likeability and attractiveness will not vary as a function of target gender or version, nor will perceptions vary as a function of the Target X Version interaction.

Responses to statements will be analyzed as a function of a 2 (Version) X 2 (Traditionalism) mixed model ANOVA with repeated measures on the last factor. I expect that participants will view candidates who endorse traditionalism more positively than those who do not. In order to move forward with the main study, the critical tests must demonstrate that perceptions of the candidates do not vary as a function of version, nor should the effects of traditionalism depend on version.

Study 1

Method

Participants

I propose to recruit approximately 300 college students from a mid-sized Midwestern University.

Materials and Procedure

Experimental manipulation. Participants will be given a news report that describes the aftermath of a terrorist attack in which 10 people were killed and 30 injured after a makeshift explosive device went off in the middle of a busy city. According to the report, the investigation is still ongoing, but federal authorities are crediting the attacks to either members of ISIS or members of a White supremacist group. All other aspects of the report, besides the ingroup (White supremacist) or outgroup (ISIS) status of the attackers will be consistent across reports. After the article, to participants will indicate their perceptions of how serious the threat is on a 1 (not at all serious) to 7 (very serious) scale to serve as an attention check and manipulation check.

Next, participants will be asked to indicate their political party and will be shown two videotaped statements from male and female ostensive political candidates (the same men and women from the pilot study) in which candidates will be described as either upholding cultural traditions or not mentioning cultural traditions. Slightly different versions of each statement from the pilot study will be used to ensure participants never see the same statement twice. Therefore, the proposed design is a 2 (Target Gender) X 2 (Threat Group: Ingroup member vs. Outgroup member) X 2 (Traditionalism: Present vs. not) X 2 (Participant Gender) design with target gender varying within subjects.

Dependent variables. After each video, participants will indicate their perceptions of how likeable each candidate is, how effective they perceive each candidate to be, how likely they would be to vote for each candidate in the presidential primary, and how likely they think each candidate might be to win the presidential election on a 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much) scale.

Finally, participants will answer a series of demographics questions such as their gender and race.

Proposed Analyses

I propose to run univariate statistics to assess for skew and kurtosis for the dependent measures. I also propose to evaluate the factor structure of the dependent measure using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, as it is possible that a multi-factor structure (e.g., likelihood of voting for the candidate in the primary and election) may load on a different factor from perceptions of candidates’ likeability or competence. Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) will also be assessed.

Next all responses will be analyzed as a 2 (Target Gender) X 2 (Threat Group: Ingroup member vs. Outgroup member) X 2 (Traditionalism: Present vs. Not) X 2 (Participant Gender) ANOVA with repeated measures on the first factor. I propose to analyze the dependent variable both as a composite scale and as single item measures, as it is possible that analysis of single item measures may yield somewhat different results from analysis of the composite scale. If participant gender yields no significant main effects or interactions, I propose dropping it as a factor from the design to improve power. I also propose to test the effects of participant ethnicity, although I have no formal hypotheses regarding participant ethnicity.

Discussion and Implications

This investigation will seek to determine the types of conditions in which female leaders might be perceived as more attractive than male leaders, specifically when a threat to the system was posed by outgroup rather than ingroup members, and when candidates signaled a return to the status quo (i,e., upheld cultural traditions). This study would contribute to the literature in a few ways.

First, there is relatively little literature on the glass cliff among female political leaders and it would therefore contribute new understanding to the conditions in which women might be nominated to precarious political leadership positions.

Secondly, although there is evidence that system-justifying ideologies may play a role in preferences for female leaders in times of threat, this study would directly manipulate such ideologies, providing stronger evidence for its role in preferences for potential female political candidates as previous studies have measured system justification as a covariate.

Additionally, better understanding the conditions under which female candidates are more desirable more generally may contribute to greater gender parity in political representation.

Although one implication of this study is that female leaders may be more desirable when they are traditional, I am not advocating for female candidates to be “traditional” in any sense, particularly not in their endorsement of traditional gender roles. Female candidates, like male candidates should run on issues that that they consider to be important and should advocate for the public interest as they see fit. However one potential implication of this study might be that the first female president of the United States may be a conservative woman, despite progressives’ push for more gender parity. Regardless, a better understanding of the existing system may facilitate interventions that help female candidates run for office and may contribute to greater gender parity in politics and in leadership positions more generally.

Conclusion

Hillary Clinton’s call for a female president candidate is a lovely vision, but not one without potential struggles for the candidate to be. There is no doubt that despite some advantages, female leaders, and female political leaders more specifically have an uphill battle. However, there may be some situations and aspects of the candidate that increase the likelihood of a female candidate, suggesting that female candidates need not necessarily be dissuaded from running when the nation is faced with threats (e.g., military threats) that complement agency. Indeed, these may increase preference for a female candidate, who might finally shatter that “highest hardest glass ceiling.”

References

Adams, S. M., Gupta, A., & Leeth, J. D. (2009). Are female executives over-represented in precarious leadership positions? British Journal of Management, 20, 1-12.

Azvedo, F., Jost, J., & Rothmund, T. (2017). “Making American Great Again” System Justification in the U.S. Presidential Election of 2016. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 3, 231-240. doi:10.1037/tps0000122

Bauer, N. M. (2015). Emotional, sensitive, and unfit for office? Gender stereotype activation and support for female candidates. Political Psychology, 36, 691-708. doi:10.1111/pops.12186

Brown, E. R., & Diekman, A. B., (2013). Differential effects of female and male candidates on system justification: Can cracks in the glass ceiling foster complacency? European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 299-306. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1951

Brown, E. R., Diekman, A. B., & Schneider, M. C. (2011). A change will do us good: Threats diminish typical preferences for male leaders. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 930–41. doi:10.1177/0146167211403322

Bruckmüller, S., & Branscombe, N. R. (2010). The glass cliff : When and why women are selected as leaders in crisis contexts. British Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 433–452. doi:10.1348/014466609X466S94

Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.03.261

Carroll, W., Hennessey, S. M., & MacDonald, R. (2013). Is there a “glass cliff?”: Examinig the phenomenon using board of director appointments in Canada. Allied Acamdies International Conference, 18, 9-13.

Clinton H. R. (2016). Hillary Clinton’s full concession speech (full text). CNN. Retrieved November 25, 2017, from: http://www.cnn.com/2016/11/09/politics/hillary-clinton-concession-speech/index.html

Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. doi:10.1177/0146167200262001

Dolan, K. (2009). The impact of gender stereotyped evaluations on support for women candidates. Political Behavior, 32, 69-88. doi:10.1007/s11109-009-9090-4

Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L., L. (2003). A female leadership advantage: An evaluation of the evidence. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 807-834. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.09.004

Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.109.3.573

Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Konsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of Leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3-22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.3

Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: An appraisal. In T. Ecks & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123-174). Marwah, NJ: Ehrlbaum.

Foschi, M. (2000). Double standards for competence: Theory and research. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 21–42.

Fridkin K., Kenney, P., & Woodall, G. (2009). Bad for men, better for women: The impact of stereotypes during negative campaigns. Political Behavior, 31, 53-77. doi:10.1007/s11109-008-9065-x

Gartzia, L., Ryan, M. K., Balluerka, N., & Aritzeta, A. (2012). Think crisis-think female: Further evidence. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 31, 603-628. doi:10.1080/1359432X.2011.591572

Gervais, S. J., & Hillard, A. L. (2011). A role congruity perspective on prejudice towards Hillary Clinton and Sarah Palin. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 11, 221-240. doi:10.111/j.1530-2415.2011.01263.x

Guo, J. (2016). A new theory for why Trump voters are so angry – that actually makes sense. The Washington Post. Retrieved November 25, 2017, from https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/11/08/a-new-theory-for-why-trump-voters-are-so-angry-that-actually-makes-sense/?utm_term=.f4a7ff904df7

Hall, R. J., Workman, J. W., & Marchioro, C. A. (1998). Sex, task, and Behavioral flexibility effects on leadership perceptions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 74, 1-32. doi:10.1006/obhd.1998.2754 

Haslam, S. A., Ryan, M. K., Kulich, C., Trojanaowski, G., & Atkins, C. (2010). Investing with prejudice: The relationship between women’s presence on company boards and objective and subjective measures of company performance. British Journal of Management, 21, 484-497. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00670.x

Haslam, S. A., & Ryan, M. K. (2008). The road to the glass cliff: Differences in the perceived suitability of men and women for leadership positions in succeeding and failing organizations. The Leadership Quarterly, 19, 530–546. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.07.011

Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., Kteily, N., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2015). Social dominance orientation: Revisiting the structure and function of a variable predicting social and political attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

38, 583-606. doi:10.1177/0146167211432765

Huddy, L. & Terkildsen, N. (1993). The consequences of gender stereotypes for women candidates at different types of office. Political Research Quarterly, 46, 503-525.

Inglehart, R. F., & Norris, P. (2016). Trump, Brexit and the rise of popular economic have-nots and cultural backlast. Retrieved November 25, 2017 from: https://poseidon01.ssrn.com/delivery.php?ID=716097073084075015119121065111120004042021055002019085109023122093009013126104004018021054116024117056041070097027096098083076055058046000054080105101067090112104032073024027126068087008120125100092068094126009097027098092118030113066006110083069096&EXT=pdf

Jost, J. T., & Hunyady, O. (2005). Antecedents and consequences of system-justifying ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 114–118.

Jost, J. T., & Thompson, E. P. (2000). Group-based dominance and opposition to equality as independent predictors of self-esteem, ethnocentrism, and social policy attitudes among African Ameri-cans and European Americans. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 209–232.

Junker, N. M., & van Dick, R. (2014). Implicit theories in organizational settings: A systematic review and research agenda of implicit leadership and followership theories. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 1154-1173. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.09.002

Kahn, K. F. (1994). Experimental examination of sex stereotypes and press patterns in statewide campaigns. American Journal of Political Science, 38, 162-195. doi:10.2307/2111340

Kark, R., Waismel-Manor, R., & Shamir, B. (2012). Does valuing androgyny and femininity lead to a female advantage? The relationship between gender-role, transformational leadership and identification. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 620–640. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.12.012

Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2014). Evidence for the social role theory of stereotype content: Observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 371–392. doi:10.1037/a0037215

Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 616–42. doi:10.1037/a0023557

Lilla, M. (2016). The end of identity liberalism. The New York Times. Retrieved November 25, 2017, from: https://www.nytimes.com/2016/11/20/opinion/sunday/the-end-of-identity-liberalism.html

Nowraseth, A. (2016). Terrorism and Immigration: A Risk Analysis. Policy Analysis, 798. Retrieved November 25, 2017 from https://object.cato.org/sites/cato.org/files/pubs/pdf/pa798_2.pdf.

Paustian-Underdahl, S. C., Walker, L. S., & Woehr, D. J. (2014). Gender and Perceptions of Leadership Effectiveness : A Meta-Analysis of Contextual Moderators, The Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 1129–1145. doi: 10.1037/a0036751

Payne, K. B. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 181-192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181

Powell, K. A. (2011). Framing Islam: An analysis of U.S. media coverage of terrorism since 9/11. Communication Studies, 62, 90-112. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2011.533599

Rosette, A. S., & Tost, L. P. (2010). Agentic women and communal leadership: how role prescriptions confer advantage to top women leaders. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(2), 221–35. doi:10.1037/a0018204

Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629

Ryan, M. K., & Haslam, S. A. (2005). The glass cliff: Evidence that women are over-represented in precarious leadership positions. British Journal of Management, 16, 81–90. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8551.2005.00433.x

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Hersby, M. D., & Bongiorno, R. (2011). Think crisis-think female: The glass cliff and contextual variation in the think manager-think male stereotype. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 470–84. doi:10.1037/a0022133

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., & Kulich, C. (2010). Politics and the glass cliff: Evidence that women are preferentially selected to contest hard-to-win seats. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34, 56-64. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01541.x

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Morgenroth, T., Rink, F., Stoker, J., & Peters, K. (2016). Getting on top of the glass cliff: reviewing a decade of evidence, explanations, and impact. The Leadership Quarterly, 27, 446-455. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.10.008

Schein, V. E. (1973). The relationship between sex role stereotypes and requisite management characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 95-100. doi:10.1037/h0037128 

Schneider, M. C. & Bos, A. L. (2014). Measuring stereotypes of female politicians. Political Psychology, 35, 245-266. doi:10.1111/pops.12040

Suliman, A. (2017). Hillary Clinton blames election loss on sexism during UK book tour. Reuters. Retrieved November 25, 2017 from: https://www.reuters.com/article/britain-culture-clinton/hillary-clinton-blames-election-loss-on-sexism-during-uk-book-tour-idUSL8N1MO4H6

Tumulty, K., Rucker, P., & Gearan, A. (2016). Donald Trump wins the presidency in stunning upset over Clinton. The Washington Post. Retrieved November 25, 2017, from https:// www.washingtonpost.com/politics/election-day-an- acrimonious-race-reaches-its-end-point/2016/11/08/

Vinkenburg, C. J., van Engen, M. L., Eagly, A. H., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2011). An exploration of stereotypical beliefs about leadership styles: Is transformational leadership a route to women’s promotion? The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 10–21. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.12.003

Willis, H. H. (2017). The wrong terrorism narrative. The Rand Blog. Retrieved November 25, 2017, from https://www.rand.org/blog/2017/01/the-wrong-terrorism-narrative.html

Wolfram, H.-J., & Gratton, L. (2013). Gender Role Self-Concept, Categorical Gender, and Transactional-Transformational Leadership: Implications for Perceived Workgroup Performance. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 21, 338–353. doi:10.1177/1548051813498421

Wood, W., Christensen, P. N., Hebl, M. R., & Rothgerber, H. (1997). Conformity to sex-typed norms, affect, and the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 523–535.

Cite This Work

To export a reference to this article please select a referencing stye below:

Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.
Reference Copied to Clipboard.

Related Services

View all

Related Content

All Tags

Content relating to: "Leadership"

Leadership can be defined as an individual or group of people influencing others to work towards a common goal. A good leader will be motivational and supportive, getting the best out of others when trying to achieve their objectives.

Related Articles

DMCA / Removal Request

If you are the original writer of this literature review and no longer wish to have your work published on the UKDiss.com website then please: